Abstract
Contrasting brain states when subjects are aware compared to unaware of a presented stimulus has allowed researchers to isolate candidate neural correlates of consciousness. Here we propose that an important next step in this research program is to investigate, perhaps paradoxically, brain states that covary with reports of absences of awareness. Specifically, we propose that in order to distinguish content-specific and content-invariant neural correlates of consciousness, a distinction needs to be made between the neural correlates of awareness of stimulus absence, and the neural correlates of absence of awareness (of either stimulus presence or absence). We ground this distinction in higher-order computational models of consciousness, where the state of higher-order nodes is invariant to the specific contents of awareness. To map the different levels of these models to neurophysiological correlates, we suggest two empirical approaches – inverted designs and two-dimensional awareness reports – in which reports about awareness and stimulus presence can be dissociated.
References
Aru, J., Bachmann, T., Singer, W., & Melloni, L. (2012). Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 737–746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.003
Aru, J., Suzuki, M., Rutiku, R., Larkum, M. E., & Bachmann, T. (2019). Coupling the state and contents of consciousness. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, 43. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00043
Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press.
Bayne, T., & Hohwy, J. (2013). Consciousness: Theoretical approaches. In A. E. Cavanna, A. Nani, H. Blumenfeld, & S. Laureys (Eds.), Neuroimaging of Consciousness (pp. 23–35). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Binder, M., Gociewicz, K., Windey, B., Koculak, M., Finc, K., Nikadon, J., Derda, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2017). The levels of perceptual processing and the neural correlates of increasing subjective visibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 55, 106–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.07.010
Boly, M., Massimini, M., Tsuchiya, N., Postle, B. R., Koch, C., & Tononi, G. (2017). Are the neural correlates of consciousness in the front or in the back of the cerebral cortex? Clinical and Neuroimaging Evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(40), 9603–9613. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3218-16.2017
Christensen, M. S., Ramsøy, T. Z., Lund, T. E., Madsen, K. H., & Rowe, J. B. (2006). An fMRI study of the neural correlates of graded visual perception. NeuroImage, 31(4), 1711–1725. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.023
Davidson, M. J., Mithen, W., Hogendoorn, H., van Boxtel, J. J., & Tsuchiya, N. (2020). The SSVEP tracks attention, not consciousness, during perceptual filling-in. eLife, 9, e60031. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60031
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (2011). Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., & Changeux, J.-P. (2003). A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(14), 8520–8525. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1332574100
Del Cul, A., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Brain dynamics underlying the nonlinear threshold for access to consciousness. PLoS Biology, 5(10), e260. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050260
Del Cul, A., Dehaene, S., Reyes, P., Bravo, E., & Slachevsky, A. (2009). Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain, 132(9), 2531–2540. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp111
Fleming, S. M. (2020). Awareness as inference in a higher-order state space. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2020(1), niz020. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niz020
Förster, J., Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2020). ERP and MEG correlates of visual consciousness: The second decade. Consciousness and Cognition, 80, 102917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.102917
Frassle, S., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Naber, M., & Einhauser, W. (2014). Binocular rivalry: Frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 1738–1747. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014
Graziano, M. S. A. (2013). Consciousness and the Social Brain. Oxford University Press.
Graziano, M. S. A., & Webb, T. W. (2015). The attention schema theory: A mechanistic account of subjective awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 06. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00500
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. John Wiley.
Helmholtz, H. von. (1948). Concerning the perceptions in general, 1867. In W. Dennis (Ed.), Readings in the history of psychology (pp. 214–230). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kanai, R., Walsh, V., & Tseng, C.-h. (2010). Subjective discriminability of invisibility: A framework for distinguishing perceptual and attentional failures of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1045–1057. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.003
Kellij, S., Fahrenfort, J., Lau, H., Peters, M. A. K., & Odegaard, B. (2020). An investigation of how relative precision of target encoding influences metacognitive performance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02190-0
King, J.-R., & Dehaene, S. (2014). A model of subjective report and objective discrimination as categorical decisions in a vast representational space. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1641), 20130204. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0204
Klein, C., & Barron, A. B. (2020). How experimental neuroscientists can fix the hard problem of consciousness. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2020(1), niaa009. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niaa009
Ko, Y., & Lau, H. (2012). A detection theoretic explanation of blindsight suggests a link between conscious perception and metacognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1401–1411. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0380
Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22
Lau, H. (2019). Consciousness, metacognition, & perceptual reality monitoring. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ckbyf
Lau, H. C. (2007). A higher order Bayesian decision theory of consciousness. In Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 168, pp. 35–48). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68004-2
Lau, H. C., & Passingham, R. E. (2006). Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(49), 18763–18768. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0607716103
Lau, H., & Rosenthal, D. (2011). Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 365–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009
Mazor, M., Friston, K. J., & Fleming, S. M. (2020). Distinct neural contributions to metacognition for detecting, but not discriminating visual stimuli. eLife, 9, e53900. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.53900
Metzinger, T. (2020). Minimal phenomenal experience: Meditation, tonic alertness, and the phenomenology of “pure” consciousness. Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 1(I), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2020.I.46
Meuwese, J. D. I., Loon, A. M. van, Lamme, V. A. F., & Fahrenfort, J. J. (2014). The subjective experience of object recognition: Comparing metacognition for object detection and object categorization. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0643-1
Odegaard, B., Knight, R. T., & Lau, H. (2017). Should a few null findings falsify prefrontal theories of conscious perception? The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(40), 9593–9602. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3217-16.2017
Pitts, M. A., Martínez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2012). Visual processing of contour patterns under conditions of inattentional blindness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(2), 287–303. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00111
Ramsøy, T. Z., & Overgaard, M. (2004). Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:PHEN.0000041900.30172.e8
Rutiku, R., Aru, J., & Bachmann, T. (2016). General markers of conscious visual perception and their timing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00023
Sandberg, K., & Overgaard, M. (2015). Using the perceptual awareness scale (PAS). In M. Overgaard (Ed.), Behavioral Methods in Consciousness Research (pp. 181–196). Oxford University Press.
Sandved Smith, L., Hesp, C., Lutz, A., Mattout, J., Friston, K., & Ramstead, M. (2020). Towards a formal neurophenomenology of metacognition: Modelling meta-awareness, mental action, and attentional control with deep active inference. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5jh3c
Simons, J. S., Garrison, J. R., & Johnson, M. K. (2017). Brain Mechanisms of Reality Monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 462–473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.012
Vallesi, A. (2012). Organisation of executive functions: Hemispheric asymmetries. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 367–386. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.678992
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Copyright (c) 2020 Matan Mazor, Stephen M. Fleming