Molyneux’s answer: Situated predictive processing
PDF
HTML

Keywords

4E cognition
Mental representations
Molyneux's problem
Predictive processing
Situated predictive processing

How to Cite

Molyneux’s answer: Situated predictive processing. (2024). Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 5. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2024.11600

Abstract

Molyneux’s problem asks whether a person blind from birth, upon gaining sight, could immediately recognize and distinguish objects by sight alone that were previously known only by touch. Historical and contemporary empirical studies have explored this question with inconclusive results due to empirical limitations. More recently, Held and colleagues (2011) found that treated congenitally blind individuals cannot immediately recognize objects previously familiar through touch. Piller and colleagues (2023) further reported the absence of visual illusions in blind and recently visually-restored individuals. Nevertheless, cross-modal mappings gradually develop post-sight restoration. These findings suggest a reluctance of the mind to make cross-modal inferences, aligning with the predictive processing (PP) framework. PP posits that the mind generates top-down predictions about sensory stimuli, updating internal models through prediction errors when expectations are not met. With no prior visual experience, generative models in congenital blind individuals fail to produce accurate predictions. PP’s representational claims have been challenged by 4E cognitivists, who emphasize embodied, embedded, extended, and enactive aspects of cognition. This paper proposes a Situated Predictive Processing (SPP) framework that integrates PP with 4E cognition through the concept of situated mental representations, offering a new perspective on the Molyneux’s problem and emphasizing the role of experience and situatedness in the gradual development of visual-tactile mappings post-sight restoration.

PDF
HTML

References

Adams, R. A., Shipp, S., & Friston, K. J. (2013). Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function, 218(3), 611–643. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5

Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Bermpohl, F., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). The occipital cortex in the blind: Lessons about plasticity and vision. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 306–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00387.x

Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. The MIT Press.

Anderson, M. L. (2017). Of Bayes and bullets: An embodied, situated, targeting-based account of predictive processing. Philosophy and Predictive Processing. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958573055

Anderson, M. L., & Chemero, A. (2013). The problem with barin GUTs: Conflation of different senses of ”perception” threatens metaphysical desaster. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1200221x

Apps, M. A. J., & Tsakiris, M. (2014). The free-energy self: A predictive coding account of self-recognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 41, 85–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.029

Bach-Y-Rita, P., Collins, C. C., Saunders, F. A., White, B., & Scadden, L. (1969). Vision substitution by tactile image projection. Nature, 221(5184), 963–964. https://doi.org/10.1038/221963a0

Badcock, P. B., Friston, K. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2019). The hierarchically mechanistic mind: A free-energy formulation of the human psyche. Physics of Life Reviews, 31, 104–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2018.10.002

Barrett, L. F. (2016). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsw154. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw154

Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950

Başoğlu, Y. R. (2021). How not to argue about the compatibility of predictive processing and 4E cognition. Organon F, 2021(4), 777–801. https://doi.org/10.31577/orgf.2021.28402

Bays, P. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (2007). Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability. The Journal of Physiology, 578(2), 387–396. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2006.120121

Bear, M. F., & Malenka, R. C. (1994). Synaptic plasticity: LTP and LTD. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4(3), 389–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(94)90101-5

Bermúdez, J. L. (2010). Cognitive science: An introduction to the science of the mind (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511781322

Bliss, T. V. P., & Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model of memory: Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature, 361(6407), 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1038/361031a0

Bliss, T. V. P., & Gardner‐Medwin, A. R. (1973). Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of Physiology, 232(2), 357–374. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010274

Bonini, L., Rotunno, C., Arcuri, E., & Gallese, V. (2022). Mirror neurons 30 years later: Implications and applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(9), 767–781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.003

Brodski, A., Paasch, G.-F., Helbling, S., & Wibral, M. (2015). The faces of predictive coding. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(24), 8997–9006. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1529-14.2015

Brodski-Guerniero, A., Paasch, G.-F., Wollstadt, P., Özdemir, I., Lizier, J. T., & Wibral, M. (2017). Information-theoretic evidence for predictive coding in the face-processing system. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37 (34), 8273–8283. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0614-17.2017

Brown, H., Friston, K., & Bestmann, S. (2011). Active inference, attention, and motor preparation. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00218

Bruineberg, J., Kiverstein, J., & Rietveld, E. (2018). The anticipating brain is not a scientist: The free-energy principle from an ecological-enactive perspective. Synthese, 195(6), 2417–2444. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1239-1

Bruno, M., & Mandelbaum, E. (2010). Locke’s answer to Molyneux’s thought experiment. History of Philosophy Quarterly, 27 (2), 165–180. https://www.jstor.org/stable/27809501

Carney, J. (2020). Thinking avant la lettre: A review of 4E cognition. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 4(1), 77–90. https://doi.org/10.26613/esic.4.1.172

Chanes, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2020). The predictive brain, conscious experience, and brain-related conditions. In The philosophy of science of predictive processing (pp. 159–169). Bloomsbury Academic.

Chanes, L., Baumann Wormwood, J., Betz, N., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2018). Facial expression predictions as drivers of social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(3), 380–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000108

Chemero, A. (2013). Radical embodied cognitive science. Review of General Psychology, 17 (2), 145–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032923

Cheng, T. (2015). Obstacles to testing Molyneux’s question empirically. I-Perception, 6(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669515599330

Cheselden, W. (1728). An account of some observations made by a young gentleman, who was blind, or lost his sight so early, that he had no rememberance of ever having seen, and was couch’d between 13 and 14 years of age. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 35, 447–450.

Chow, A. Y. (2004). The artificial silicon retina microchip for the treatment of vision loss from retinitis pigmentosa. Archives of Ophthalmology, 122(4), 460. https://doi.org/10.1001/archopht.122.4.460

Cisek, P. (1999). Beyond the computer metaphor: Behaviour as interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11-12), 125–142.

Clark, A. (1996). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/1552.001.0001

Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 345–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01361-3

Clark, A. (2012). Dreaming the whole cat: Generative models, predictive processing, and the enactivist conception of perceptual experience. Mind, 121(483), 753–771. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzs106

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477

Clark, A. (2015a). Predicting peace: The end of the representation wars – a reply to Michael Madary. Open MIND. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570979

Clark, A. (2015b). Radical predictive processing. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 53(S1), 3 27. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjp.12120

Clark, A. (2022). Extending the predictive mind. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 102(1), 119–130. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048402.2022.2122523

Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3328150

Clark, A., & Toribio, J. (1994). Doing without representing? Synthese, 101(3), 401–431. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01063896

Clarke, S. (2016). Investigating what felt shapes look like. I-Perception, 7 (1), 2041669515627948. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669515627948

Colombo, M., & Piccinini, G. (2023). The computational theory of mind (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009183734

Connolly, K. (2013). How to test Molyneux’s question empirically. I-Perception, 4(8), 508–510. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0623jc

Constant, A., Clark, A., Kirchhoff, M., & Friston, K. J. (2020). Extended active inference: Constructing predictive cognition beyond skulls. Mind & Language, 37 (3), 373–394. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12330

Cummins, R. (1991). The role of representation in connectionist explanations of cognitive capacities. In W. Ramsey, S. Stich, & D. Rumelhart (Eds.), Philosophy and connectionist theory (pp. 91–114). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cummins, R. (1995). Meaning and mental representation (3. print). MIT Press.

Das, S., Meyyappan, S., Ding, M., & Mangun, G. R. (2023). Top-down effects on cross-modal stimulus processing: A predictive coding framework. Journal of Vision, 23(9), 5801. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.23.9.5801

De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9

De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., & Gallagher, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441–447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009

Degenaar, M., & Collins, M. J. (1996). Molyneux’s problem: Three centuries of discussion on the perception of forms. Kluwer academic publ.

Dell’Erba, S., Brown, D. J., & Proulx, M. J. (2018). Synesthetic hallucinations induced by psychedelic drugs in a congenitally blind man. Consciousness and Cognition, 60, 127–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.008

Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology, 39(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1977.tb01600.x

Dercksen, T. T., Stuckenberg, M. V., Schröger, E., Wetzel, N., & Widmann, A. (2021). Cross‐modal predictive processing depends on context rather than local contingencies. Psychophysiology, 58(6), e13811. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13811

Deroy, O., & Auvray, M. (2012). Reading the world through the skin and ears: A new perspective on sensory substitution. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00457

Di Paolo, E. (2009). Extended life. Topoi, 28(1), 9–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-008-9042-3

Di Paolo, E., & Thompson, E. (2014). The enactive approach. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), The routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 68–78). Routledge.

Dobelle, Wm. H. (2000). Artificial vision for the blind by connecting a television camera to the visual cortex. ASAIO Journal, 46(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00002480-200001000-00002

Dokic, J. (2007). Situated representations and ad hoc concepts. In M. J. Frápolli (Ed.), Saying, meaning and referring: Essays on françois recanati’s philosophy of language (pp. 203–220). Palgrave-Macmillan.

Downey, A. (2018). Predictive processing and the representation wars: A victory for the eliminativist (via fictionalism). Synthese, 195(12), 5115–5139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1442-8

Draganov, M., Galiano-Landeira, J., Doruk Camsari, D., Ramı́rez, J.-E., Robles, M., & Chanes, L. (2023). Noninvasive modulation of predictive coding in humans: Causal evidence for frequency-specific temporal dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 33(13), 8421–8430. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhad127

Dretske, F. I. (1997). Explaining behavior: Reasons in a world of causes (5th print). MIT Press.

Egan, F. (2014). How to think about mental content. Philosophical Studies, 170(1), 115–135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-013-0172-0

Engel, A. K., Friston, K. J., & Kragic, D. (Eds.). (2016). The pragmatic turn: Toward action-oriented views in cognitive science. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262034326.001.0001

Evans, G. (1985). Collected papers. Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.

Facchin, M. (2021a). Are generative models structural representations? Minds and Machines, 31(2), 277–303. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-021-09559-6

Facchin, M. (2021b). Structural representations do not meet the job description challenge. Synthese, 199(3-4), 5479–5508. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-021-03032-8

Facchin, M. (2023). Extended predictive minds: Do Markov blankets matter? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 14(3), 909–938. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00607-9

Facchin, M. (2024). Maps, simulations, spaces and dynamics: On distinguishing types of structural representations. Erkenntnis. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-024-00831-6

Favela, L. H. (2023). The ecological brain: Unifying the sciences of brain, body, and environment (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003009955

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815–836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

Friston, K. (2009). The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 293–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787

Friston, K. (2012). The history of the future of the Bayesian brain. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1230–1233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.004

Friston, K., Mattout, J., & Kilner, J. (2011). Action understanding and active inference. Biological Cybernetics, 104(1-2), 137–160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z

Fusaroli, R., Gangopadhyay, N., & Tylén, K. (2014). The dialogically extended mind: Language as skillful intersubjective engagement. Cognitive Systems Research, 29-30, 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.06.002

Gallagher, S. (1996). First perception: A new solution to the molyneux problem. Proceedings of the New York State Philosophical Association.

Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind (1st ed.). Oxford University PressOxford. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199271941.001.0001

Gallagher, S. (2008). Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition, 17 (2), 535–543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.003

Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions (Vol. 1). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198794325.001.0001

Gandhi, T., Kalia, A., Ganesh, S., & Sinha, P. (2015). Immediate susceptibility to visual illusions after sight onset. Current Biology, 25(9), R358–R359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.005

Gangopadhyay, N., & Kiverstein, J. (2009). Enactivism and the unity of perception and action. Topoi, 28(1), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-008-9047-y

Gibson, J. J. (2015). The ecological approach to visual perception: Classic edition. Psychology Press.

Gładziejewski, P. (2016). Predictive coding and representationalism. Synthese, 193(2), 559–582. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0762-9

Glenney, B. (2011). Adam Smith and the problem of the external world. Journal of Scottish Philosophy, 9(2), 205–223. https://doi.org/10.3366/jsp.2011.0016

Glenney, B. (2012). Leibniz on Molyneux’s question. History of Philosophy Quarterly, 29(3), 247–264.

Guerreiro, M. J. S., Putzar, L., & Röder, B. (2016a). The effect of early visual deprivation on the neural bases of auditory processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(5), 1620–1630. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2559-15.2016

Guerreiro, M. J. S., Putzar, L., & Röder, B. (2016b). Persisting cross-modal changes in sight-recovery individuals modulate visual perception. Current Biology, 26(22), 3096–3100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.069

Haugeland, J. (1991). Representational genera. In W. Ramsey, S. Stich, & D. Rumelhart (Eds.), Philosophy and connectionist theory (pp. 61–89). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. Wiley.

Held, R., Ostrovsky, Y., De Gelder, B., Gandhi, T., Ganesh, S., Mathur, U., & Sinha, P. (2011). The newly sighted fail to match seen with felt. Nature Neuroscience, 14(5), 551–553. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2795

Helmholz, H. von. (1867). Treatise on physiological optics. The Optical Society of America.

Heras-Escribano, M., & Martı́nez Moreno, D. (2024). The emergence of ur-intentionality: An ecological proposal. Philosophies, 9(3), 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies9030054

Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096

Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001

Hohwy, J. (2016). The self‐evidencing brain. Noûs, 50(2), 259–285. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12062

Hohwy, J. (2018). The predictive processing hypothesis. In The oxford handbook of 4E cognition (pp. 161–197). Oxford University Press.

Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 160(1), 106–154. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837

Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 195(1), 215–243. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455

Huntenburg, J. M., Bazin, P.-L., & Margulies, D. S. (2018). Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.002

Hutto, D. (2022). Getting real about pretense: A radical enactivist proposal. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 21(5), 1157–1175. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09826-6

Hutto, D. D., Gallagher, S., Ilundáin-Agurruza, J., & Hipólito, I. (2020). Culture in mind – an enactivist account: Not cognitive penetration but cultural permeation. In L. J. In L. J. Kirmayer, C. M. Worthman, S. Kitayama, R. Lemelson, & C. & Cummings (Eds.), Culture, mind, and brain: Emerging concepts, models, and applications (pp. 163–187). Cambridge University Press.

Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. MIT Press.

Jacomuzzi, A. C., Kobau, P., & Bruno, N. (2003). Molyneux’ question redux. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 255–280. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:PHEN.0000007370.68536.2d

Kandel, E. R. (Ed.). (2013). Principles of neural science (5th ed). McGraw-Hill.

Kant, I., & Hatfield, G. C. (2005). Prolegomena to any future metaphysics that will be able to come forward as science: With selections from the Critique of pure reason (Rev. ed., reprinted). Cambridge Univ. Press.

Kersten, L. (2022). A new mark of the cognitive? Predictive processing and extended cognition. Synthese, 200(4), 281. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03674-2

Kersten, L., & Philosophy Documentation Center. (2024). Recruitment revisited in advance: Cognitive extension and the promise of predictive processing. Thought: A Journal of Philosophy. https://doi.org/10.5840/tht202492035

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., Feusner, J. D., Garfinkel, S. N., Lane, R. D., Mehling, W. E., Meuret, A. E., Nemeroff, C. B., Oppenheimer, S., Petzschner, F. H., Pollatos, O., Rhudy, J. L., Schramm, L. P., Simmons, W. K., Stein, M. B., … Zucker, N. (2018). Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(6), 501–513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004

Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2

Kirchhoff, M. D., & Kiverstein, J. (2021). How to determine the boundaries of the mind: A Markov blanket proposal. Synthese, 198(5), 4791–4810. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02370-y

Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., Quigley, K. S., Dickerson, B. C., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5), 0069. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0069

Lee, J., & Calder, D. (2023). The many problems with S-representation (and how to solve them). Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 4. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2023.9758

Lettvin, J., Maturana, H., McCulloch, W., & Pitts, W. (1959). What the frog’s eye tells the frog’s brain. Proceedings of the IRE, 47 (11), 1940–1951. https://doi.org/10.1109/JRPROC.1959.287207

Lewis, P. M., Ackland, H. M., Lowery, A. J., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2015). Restoration of vision in blind individuals using bionic devices: A review with a focus on cortical visual prostheses. Brain Research, 1595, 51–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.020

Loaiza, J. R. (2020). Accessibility and phenomenality: Remarks on solving molyneux’s question empirically. Humanitas Hodie, 2(2), h223. https://doi.org/10.28970/hh.2019.2.a3

Maurer, D., Stager, C. L., & Mondloch, C. J. (1999). Cross‐modal transfer of shape is difficult to demonstrate in one‐month‐olds. Child Development, 70(5), 1047–1057. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00077

Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282(5737), 403–404. https://doi.org/10.1038/282403a0

Menary, R. (2010). Introduction to the special issue on 4E cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 459–463. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9187-6

Miłkowski, M. (2017). Situatedness and embodiment of computational systems. Entropy, 19(4), 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/e19040162

Millidge, B., Seth, A., & Buckley, C. L. (2021). Predictive coding: A theoretical and experimental review. arXiv. https://doi.org/10.48550/ARXIV.2107.12979

Morgan, A. (2014). Representations gone mental. Synthese, 191(2), 213–244. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-013-0328-7

Najarpour Foroushani, A., Pack, C. C., & Sawan, M. (2018). Cortical visual prostheses: From microstimulation to functional percept. Journal of Neural Engineering, 15(2), 021005. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2552/aaa904

Nau, A., Murphy, M., & Chan, K. (2015). Use of sensory substitution devices as a model system for investigating cross-modal neuroplasticity in humans. Neural Regeneration Research, 10(11), 1717. https://doi.org/10.4103/1673-5374.169612

Nave, K. (2025). A drive to survive: The free energy principle and the meaning of life. The MIT Press.

Neal, R. M., & Hinton, G. E. (1998). A view of the EM algorithm that justifies incremental, sparse, and other variants. In M. I. Jordan (Ed.), Learning in Graphical Models (pp. 355–368). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-5014-9_12

Newen, A., De Bruin, L., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.001.0001

Newen, A., & Vosgerau, G. (2020). Situated mental representations: Why we need mental representations and how we should understand them. In J. Smortchkova, K. Dołęga, & T. Schlicht (Eds.), What are Mental Representations? (1st ed., pp. 178–212). Oxford University Press New York. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190686673.003.0007

Nirshberg, G., & Shapiro, L. (2021). Structural and indicator representations: A difference in degree, not kind. Synthese, 198(8), 7647–7664. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02537-y

Noë, A. (2009). Out of our heads: Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness (1. ed). Hill; Wang.

O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000115

Ohata, W., & Tani, J. (2020). Investigation of the sense of agency in social cognition, based on frameworks of predictive coding and active inference: A simulation study on multimodal imitative interaction. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 14, 61. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbot.2020.00061

Orlandi, N. (2016). Bayesian perception is ecological perception. Philosophical Topics, 44(2), 327–352. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26529415

Ostrovsky, Y., Meyers, E., Ganesh, S., Mathur, U., & Sinha, P. (2009). Visual parsing after recovery from blindness. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1484–1491. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02471.x

Owens, A. P., Friston, K. J., Low, D. A., Mathias, C. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2018). Investigating the relationship between cardiac interoception and autonomic cardiac control using a predictive coding framework. Autonomic Neuroscience, 210, 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2018.01.001

Pant, R., Guerreiro, M. J. S., Ley, P., Bottari, D., Shareef, I., Kekunnaya, R., & Röder, B. (2021). The size-weight illusion is unimpaired in individuals with a history of congenital visual deprivation. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 6693. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86227-w

Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning, and inference (Second edition, reprinted with corrections). Cambridge University Press.

Peelen, M. V., He, C., Han, Z., Caramazza, A., & Bi, Y. (2014). Nonvisual and visual object shape representations in occipitotemporal cortex: Evidence from congenitally blind and sighted adults. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(1), 163–170. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1114-13.2014

Pessoa, L. (2014). Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Physics of Life Reviews, 11(3), 400–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2014.03.005

Petkova, V. I., Zetterberg, H., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Rubber hands feel touch, but not in blind individuals. PLoS ONE, 7 (4), e35912. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035912

Petzschner, F. H., Garfinkel, S. N., Paulus, M. P., Koch, C., & Khalsa, S. S. (2021). Computational models of interoception and body regulation. Trends in Neurosciences, 44(1), 63–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.09.012

Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., & Friston, K. (2015). Active inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Progress in Neurobiology, 134, 17–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001

Pezzulo, G., Zorzi, M., & Corbetta, M. (2021). The secret life of predictive brains: What’s spontaneous activity for? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(9), 730–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.05.007

Piccinini, G. (2022). Situated neural representations: Solving the problems of content. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 16, 846979. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbot.2022.846979

Pierce, C. S. (1931). The collected papers of C. S. Pierce. Cambridge University Press.

Piller, S., Senna, I., & Ernst, M. O. (2023). Visual experience shapes the Bouba-Kiki effect and the size-weight illusion upon sight restoration from congenital blindness. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 11435. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-38486-y

Putzar, L., Goerendt, I., Lange, K., Rösler, F., & Röder, B. (2007). Early visual deprivation impairs multisensory interactions in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 10(10), 1243–1245. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1978

Putzar, L., Hötting, K., & Röder, B. (2010). Early visual deprivation affects the development of face recognition and of audio-visual speech perception. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28(2), 251–257. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2010-0526

Ramirez, K. A., Drew-Bear, L. E., Vega-Garces, M., Betancourt-Belandria, H., & Arevalo, J. F. (2023). An update on visual prosthesis. International Journal of Retina and Vitreous, 9(1), 73. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40942-023-00498-1

Ramos-Grille, I., Weyant, J., Wormwood, J. B., Robles, M., Vallès, V., Camprodon, J. A., & Chanes, L. (2022). Predictive processing in depression: Increased prediction error following negative valence contexts and influence of recent mood-congruent yet irrelevant experiences. Journal of Affective Disorders, 311, 8–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.05.030

Ramsey, W. (2016). Untangling two questions about mental representation. New Ideas in Psychology, 40, 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.01.004

Ramsey, W. (2017). Must cognition be representational? Synthese, 194(11), 4197–4214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0644-6

Ramsey, W. M. (2007). Representation reconsidered (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511597954

Reich, L., Maidenbaum, S., & Amedi, A. (2012). The brain as a flexible task machine: Implications for visual rehabilitation using noninvasive vs. Invasive approaches. Current Opinion in Neurology, 25(1), 86–95. https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834ed723

Ricciardi, E., Bonino, D., Sani, L., Vecchi, T., Guazzelli, M., Haxby, J. V., Fadiga, L., & Pietrini, P. (2009). Do we really need vision? How blind people “see” the actions of others. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 9719–9724. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0274-09.2009

Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2014). Neurocognitive mechanisms of perception–action coordination: A review and theoretical integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 46, 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.008

Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Emotion in action: A predictive processing perspective and theoretical synthesis. Emotion Review, 9(4), 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916661765

Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Brass, M. (2015). How kinesthetic motor imagery works: A predictive-processing theory of visualization in sports and motor expertise. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 109(1-3), 53–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2015.02.003

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27 (1), 169–192. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

Roth, M. A. (2010). Representation, philosophical issues about. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1(1), 32–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.31

Rowlands, M. (2010). The new science of the mind: From extended mind to embodied phenomenology. MIT Press.

Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & AU. (1986a). Parallel distributed processing, volume 1: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Foundations. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/5236.001.0001

Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & AU. (1986b). Parallel distributed processing, volume 2: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Psychological and biological models. MIT Press.

Rutar, D., Wiese, W., & Kwisthout, J. (2022). From representations in predictive processing to degrees of representational features. Minds and Machines, 32(3), 461–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-022-09599-6

Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibañez, V., Deiber, M.-P., Dold, G., & Hallett, M. (1996). Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature, 380(6574), 526–528. https://doi.org/10.1038/380526a0

Sánchez-Garcı́a, C., Alsius, A., Enns, J. T., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2011). Cross-modal prediction in speech perception. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e25198. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025198

Sassen, B. (2004). Kant on Molyneux’s problem. British Journal for the History of Philosophy, 12(3), 471–485. https://doi.org/10.1080/0960878042000253114

Schulkin, J., & Sterling, P. (2019). Allostasis: A brain-centered, predictive mode of physiological regulation. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(10), 740–752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.010

Schwenkler, J. (2012). On the matching of seen and felt shape by newly sighted subjects. I-Perception, 3(3), 186–188. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0525ic

Schwenkler, J. (2013). Do things look the way they feel? Analysis, 73(1), 86–96. https://doi.org/10.1093/analys/ans137

Schwenkler, J. (2015). Long-term deprivation affects visual perception and cortex. Frontiers in Psychology.

Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17 (11), 565–573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007

Seth, A. K. (2014). A predictive processing theory of sensorimotor contingencies: Explaining the puzzle of perceptual presence and its absence in synesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(2), 97–118. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.877880

Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395

Seth, A. K., & Tsakiris, M. (2018). Being a beast machine: The somatic basis of selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 969–981. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.008

Seth, A., & Friston, K. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1708). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0007

Shagrir, O. (2012). Structural representations and the brain. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 63(3), 519–545. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axr038

Shapiro, L. A. (2011). Embodied cognition (1. publ). Routledge.

Sprevak, M. (2011). William M. Ramsey representation reconsidered. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 62(3), 669–675. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axr022

Stewart, J., Gapenne, O., & Di Paolo, E. A. (2014). Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. the MIT press.

Swanson, L. R. (2016). The predictive processing paradigm has roots in Kant. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2016.00079

Swoyer, C. (1991). Structural representation and surrogative reasoning. Synthese, 87, 449–508.

Telakivi, P. (2023). Extending the extended mind: From cognition to consciousness (1st ed). Springer International Publishing AG.

Thompson, E. (2010). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind (First Harvard University Press paperback edition). The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Tschantz, A., Seth, A. K., & Buckley, C. L. (2020). Learning action-oriented models through active inference. PLOS Computational Biology, 16(4), e1007805. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007805

Van Es, T. (2020). Minimizing prediction errors in predictive processing: From inconsistency to non-representationalism. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 19(5), 997–1017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-019-09649-y

Varela, F. J., Rosch, E., & Thompson, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (2016). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience (revised edition). MIT Press.

Vilarroya, O. (2017). Neural representation. A survey-based analysis of the notion. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1458. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01458

Von Eckardt, B. (2012). The representational theory of mind. In K. Frankish & W. Ramsey (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognitive science (pp. 29–50). Cambridge University Press.

Wade, N. (2020). Molyneux’s vision. Routledge.

Ward, J. (2013). Synesthesia. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 49–75. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143840

Ward, J., & Meijer, P. (2010). Visual experiences in the blind induced by an auditory sensory substitution device. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 492–500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.10.006

Wiese, W. (2017). What are the contents of representations in predictive processing? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 715–736. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-016-9472-0

Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1965). Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit response in kittens. Journal of Neurophysiology, 28(6), 1029–1040. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1965.28.6.1029

Williams, D. (2018). Predictive processing and the representation wars. Minds and Machines, 28(1), 141–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-017-9441-6

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright (c) 2024 Jordi Galiano-Landeira