Abstract
The flow of thought is persistent, and at times merciless. Mental content is generated throughout the day and into the night, moving forward predictably at times but surprisingly at others. Understanding what influences the trajectory of thought—how thoughts continuously unfold over time—has important implications for the diagnosis and treatment of thought disorders like schizophrenia and recurrent nightmares. Here, we examine whether cognitive control restricts moment-to-moment content shifts across sleep and wakefulness, thus acting as a fundamental constraint on thought variability. Thought variability was measured as the semantic incoherence between sequential thought phrases and was applied to independent datasets of dreaming and waking reports. Our results show that within both sleeping and waking reports, conditions typically marked by higher levels of cognitive control were associated with decreased thought variability (i.e., semantic incoherence). During wakefulness, on-task conditions were associated with reduced levels of thought variability compared to off-task conditions, and thought variability was greater when thoughts wandered around more freely. During sleep, lucid dreams, marked by higher levels of cognitive control, were associated with reduced levels of thought variability compared to non-lucid dreams. Together, these results suggest that cognitive control may limit thought variability across the 24-hour cycle of thought generation. Such findings are consistent with the Dynamic Framework of Thought, where mental states are expected to vary on a continuum of deliberate constraints, with lower cognitive control leading to a categorical cluster of spontaneous thought processes that includes both mind-wandering during wakefulness and non-lucid dreams during sleep. This observation has broad implications for models of cognition, specifically highlighting the continuity of cognitive processes throughout the circadian cycle and the importance of considering varying levels of thought constraint in both waking and dreaming states.
References
Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00057-X
Alperin, B. R., Christoff, K., Mills, C., & Karalunas, S. L. (2021). More than off-task: Increased freely-moving thought in ADHD. Consciousness and Cognition, 93, 103156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103156
Baird, B., Mota-Rolim, S. A., & Dresler, M. (2019). The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 305–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.008
Bakdash, J. Z., & Marusich, L. R. (2017). Repeated measures correlation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 456. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00456
Bedi, G., Carrillo, F., Cecchi, G. A., Slezak, D. F., Sigman, M., Mota, N. B., Ribeiro, S., Javitt, D. C., Copelli, M., & Corcoran, C. M. (2015). Automated analysis of free speech predicts psychosis onset in high-risk youths. Npj Schizophrenia, 1(1), 15030. https://doi.org/10.1038/npjschz.2015.30
Boer, J. de, Voppel, A., Begemann, M., Schnack, H., Wijnen, F., & Sommer, I. (2018). Clinical use of semantic space models in psychiatry and neurology: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 93, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.008
Brosowsky, N. P., Murray, S., Schooler, J. W., & Seli, P. (2021). Thought dynamics under task demands: Evaluating the influence of task difficulty on unconstrained thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47(9), 1298–1312. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000944
Carr, M., Blanchette-Carrière, C., Solomonova, E., Paquette, T., & Nielsen, T. (2016). Intensified daydreams and nap dreams in frequent nightmare sufferers. Dreaming, 26(2), 119–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000024
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Corcoran, C. M., & Cecchi, G. A. (2020). Using language processing and speech analysis for the identification of psychosis and other disorders. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(8), 770–779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.06.004
Corcoran, C. M., Mittal, V. A., Bearden, C. E., E. Gur, R., Hitczenko, K., Bilgrami, Z., Savic, A., Cecchi, G. A., & Wolff, P. (2020). Language as a biomarker for psychosis: A natural language processing approach. Schizophrenia Research, 226, 158–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.04.032
Davidson, T. (2021, August 3). Haiku competition 2021. The Society of Classical Poets; The Society of Classical Poets. https://classicalpoets.org/2021/07/28/haiku-competition-2021/#comment-479800
Dijkstra, N., Kok, P., & Fleming, S. M. (2022). Perceptual reality monitoring: Neural mechanisms dissociating imagination from reality. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 135, 104557. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104557
Dream journals. (n.d.). Dream Views; Dream Views. Retrieved November 24, 2021, from https://www.dreamviews.com/blogs
Dresler, M., Koch, S. P., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Holsboer, F., Steiger, A., Sämann, P. G., Obrig, H., & Czisch, M. (2011). Dreamed movement elicits activation in the sensorimotor cortex. Current Biology, 21(21), 1833–1837. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.029
Elvevåg, B., Foltz, P. W., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2007). Quantifying incoherence in speech: An automated methodology and novel application to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 304–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.001
Feliu-Soler, A., Pérez-Aranda, A., Montero-Marín, J., Herrera-Mercadal, P., Andrés-Rodríguez, L., Angarita-Osorio, N., Williams, A. D., & Luciano, J. V. (2019). Fifteen years controlling unwanted thoughts: A systematic review of the thought control ability questionnaire (TCAQ). Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1446. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01446
Genugten, R. D. van, & Schacter, D. L. (2024). Automated scoring of the autobiographical interview with natural language processing. Behavior Research Methods, 56(3), 2243–2259. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02145-x
Gross, M., Smith, A., Graveline, Y., Beaty, R., Schooler, J., & Seli, P. (2021). Comparing the phenomenological qualities of stimulus-independent thought, stimulus-dependent thought and dreams using experience sampling. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B, 376(1817), 20190694. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0694
Kam, J. W. Y., Irving, Z. C., Mills, C., Patel, S., Gopnik, A., & Knight, R. T. (2021). Distinct electrophysiological signatures of task-unrelated and dynamic thoughts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(4), e2011796118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2011796118
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x
Kirberg, M. (2022). Neurocognitive dynamics of spontaneous offline simulations: Re-conceptualizing (dream) bizarreness. Philosophical Psychology, 0(0), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2022.2042231
Konkoly, K. R., Appel, K., Chabani, E., Mangiaruga, A., Gott, J., Mallett, R., Caughran, B., Witkowski, S., Whitmore, N. W., Mazurek, C. Y., Berent, J. B., Weber, F. D., Türker, B., Leu-Semenescu, S., Maranci, J.-B., Pipa, G., Arnulf, I., Oudiette, D., Dresler, M., & Paller, K. A. (2021). Real-time dialogue between experimenters and dreamers during REM sleep. Current Biology, 31(7), 1417–1427.e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.026
LaBerge, S., LaMarca, K., & Baird, B. (2018). Pre-sleep treatment with galantamine stimulates lucid dreaming: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. PLOS One, 13(8), e0201246. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201246
Li, H.-X., Lu, B., Chen, X., Li, X.-Y., Castellanos, F. X., & Yan, C.-G. (2021). Exploring self-generated thoughts in a resting state with natural language processing. Behavior Research Methods, 54(4), 1725–1743. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01710-6
Li, H.-X., Lu, B., Wang, Y.-W., Li, X.-Y., Chen, X., & Yan, C.-G. (2023). Neural representations of self-generated thought during think-aloud fMRI. Neuroimage, 265, 119775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119775
Mallett, R., Carr, M., Freegard, M., Konkoly, K., Bradshaw, C., & Schredl, M. (2021). Exploring the range of reported dream lucidity. Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 2(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2021.63
Mallett, R., Sowin, L., Raider, R., Konkoly, K. R., & Paller, K. A. (2022). Benefits and concerns of seeking and experiencing lucid dreams: Benefits are tied to successful induction and dream control. SLEEP Advances, 3(1), zpac027. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleepadvances/zpac027
Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2019). Spontaneous thought as an unconstrained memory process. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(11), 763–777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.09.001
Mildner, J. N., & Tamir, D. I. (2024). Why do we think? The dynamics of spontaneous thought reveal its functions. PNAS Nexus, 3(6), pgae230. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae230
Mills, C., Herrera-Bennett, A., Faber, M., & Christoff, K. (2018). Why the mind wanders: How spontaneous thought’s default variability may support episodic efficiency and semantic optimization (K. C. R. Fox & K. Christoff, Eds.; Vol. 1). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.42
Mills, C., Porter, A. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Christoff, K., & Colby, A. (2021). How task-unrelated and freely moving thought relate to affect: Evidence for dissociable patterns in everyday life. Emotion, 21(5), 1029–1040. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000849
Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., & Christoff, K. (2018). Is an off-task mind a freely-moving mind? Examining the relationship between different dimensions of thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 20–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.10.003
Montani, I., Honnibal, M., Honnibal, M., Boyd, A., Landeghem, S. V., & Peters, H. (2023). spaCy: Industrial-strength natural language processing in Python. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10009823
Mota-Rolim, S. A., Targino, Z. H., Souza, B. C., Blanco, W., Araujo, J. F., & Ribeiro, S. (2013). Dream characteristics in a Brazilian sample: An online survey focusing on lucid dreaming. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 836. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00836
Pashler, H., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 629–651. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629
Raffaelli, Q., Mills, C., De Stefano, N.-A., Mehl, M. R., Chambers, K., Fitzgerald, S. A., Wilcox, R., Christoff, K., Andrews, E. S., Grilli, M. D., O’Connor, M.-F., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2021). The think aloud paradigm reveals differences in the content, dynamics and conceptual scope of resting state thought in trait brooding. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 19362. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-98138-x
Rosen, M. G. (2013). What i make up when i wake up: Anti-experience views and narrative fabrication of dreams. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(514). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00514
Sandell, C., Stumbrys, T., Paller, K. A., & Mallett, R. (2024). Intentionally awakening from sleep through lucid dreaming. Current Psychology, 43(21), 19236–19245. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-05718-x
Saunders, D. T., Roe, C. A., Smith, G., & Clegg, H. (2016). Lucid dreaming incidence: A quality effects meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Consciousness and Cognition, 43, 197–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.06.002
Schredl, M., Fuchs, C., & Mallett, R. (2022). Differences between lucid and nonlucid dream reports: A within-subjects design. Dreaming, 32(4), 345–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000199
Schredl, M., Rieger, J., & Göritz, A. S. (2018). Measuring lucid dreaming skills: A new questionnaire (LUSK). International Journal of Dream Research, 11(1), 54–61. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2018.1.44040
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415
Siclari, F., LaRocque, J. J., Postle, B. R., & Tononi, G. (2013). Assessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(542). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542
Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9(2), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014855
Solomonova, E., & Carr, M. (2022). The role of attention and intention in dreams. In M. Wehrle, D. D’Angelo, & E. Solomonova (Eds.), Access and mediation: Transdisciplinary perspectives on attention (pp. 163–188). De Gruyter Saur. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110647242-008
Sripada, C., & Taxali, A. (2020). Structure in the stream of consciousness: Evidence from a verbalized thought protocol and automated text analytic methods. Consciousness and Cognition, 85, 103007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.103007
Stumbrys, T., & Erlacher, D. (2017). Mindfulness and lucid dream frequency predicts the ability to control lucid dreams. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 36(3), 229–239. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236616683388
Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., Johnson, M., & Schredl, M. (2014). The phenomenology of lucid dreaming: An online survey. American Journal of Psychology, 127(2), 191–204. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0191
Tononi, G., Boly, M., & Cirelli, C. (2024). Consciousness and sleep. Neuron, 112(10), 1568–1594. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2024.04.011
Türker, B., Musat, E. M., Chabani, E., Fonteix-Galet, A., Maranci, J.-B., Wattiez, N., Pouget, P., Sitt, J., Naccache, L., Arnulf, I., & Oudiette, D. (2023). Behavioral and brain responses to verbal stimuli reveal transient periods of cognitive integration of the external world during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 26(11), 1981–1993. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01449-7
Vallat, R. (2018). Pingouin: Statistics in Python. Journal of Open Source Software, 3(31), 1026. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01026
Van Den Driessche, C., Bastian, M., Peyre, H., Stordeur, C., Acquaviva, É., Bahadori, S., Delorme, R., & Sackur, J. (2017). Attentional lapses in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Blank rather than wandering thoughts. Psychological Science, 28(10), 1375–1386. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617708234
Vargha, A., & Delaney, H. D. (2000). A critique and improvement of the CL common language effect size statistics of McGraw and Wong. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 25(2), 101–132. https://doi.org/10.3102/10769986025002101
Voss, U., Frenzel, C., Koppehele-Gossel, J., & Hobson, A. (2012). Lucid dreaming: An age-dependent brain dissociation. Journal of Sleep Research, 21(6), 634–642. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01022.x
Voss, U., Schermelleh-Engel, K., Windt, J., Frenzel, C., & Hobson, A. (2013). Measuring consciousness in dreams: The lucidity and consciousness in dreams scale. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 8–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.11.001
Windt, J. M. (2013). Reporting dream experience: Why (not) to be skeptical about dream reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(708). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00708
Windt, J. M., & Voss, U. (2018). Spontaneous thought, insight, and control in lucid dreams. In K. C. R. Fox & K. Christoff (Eds.), The oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 385–410). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.26
Woods, T. J., Windt, J. M., & Carter, O. (2024). Evidence synthesis indicates contentless experiences in meditation are neither truly contentless nor identical. Phenomenology and The Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 253–304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-022-09811-z

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Copyright (c) 2025 Remington Mallett, Yasmeen Nahas, Kalina Christoff, Ken A. Paller, Caitlin Mills