Theory of mind deficits in language delayed deaf subjects? A reconsideration of evidence from false belief tasks
PDF

Keywords

Deafness
False belief
Gesture
Sign language
Social cognition
Theory of mind

How to Cite

Theory of mind deficits in language delayed deaf subjects? A reconsideration of evidence from false belief tasks. (2023). Philosophy and the Mind Sciences, 4. https://doi.org/10.33735/phimisci.2023.10269

Abstract

Recent studies claim to show that language delayed deaf subjects typically display longlingering deficits in Theory of Mind (ToM) development, despite suffering no known deficits in other cognitive domains. These claims are supported by experimental evidence indicating that such subjects fare poorly on False Belief (FB) tasks. This paper turns a critical eye on these claims. In particular, I argue that the reported results raise important questions about the status of FB tasks as evidence, and about how such evidence should be weighted against naturalistic observations of subjects engaged in everyday activities requiring complex social coordination. I conclude that these studies give us no decisive reason to believe that language delayed deaf subjects suffer distinctively and symptomatically in the domain of social cognition. To the contrary, the attribution of significant socio-cognitive impairment is potentially stigmatizing and may not help us understand the unique challenges these subjects face or suggest remedial strategies to aid them in overcoming these challenges.

PDF

References

Altschuler, M. R., & Faja, S. (2022). Brief report: Test–retest reliability of cognitive, affective, and spontaneous theory of mind tasks among school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52(4), 1890–1895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05040-6

Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016923

Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 110–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/4635.001.0001

Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen; H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2nd ed (pp. 3–20). Oxford University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

Begby, E. (2017). Language from the ground up: A study of homesign communication. Erkenntnis, 82(3), 693–714. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-016-9839-1

Bishop, M., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (2005). Symbolic play in congenitally blind children. Development and Psychopathology, 17(02), 447–465. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050212

Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77(1), B25–B31. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00096-2

Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there ”autistic-like” features in congenitally blind children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(6), 693–703. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01696.x

Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.

Carruthers, P. (2016). Two systems for mindreading? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7(1), 141–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0259-y

Caselli, N., Pyers, J., & Lieberman, A. M. (2021). Deaf children of hearing parents have age-level vocabulary growth when exposed to American Sign Language by 6 months of age. The Journal of Pediatrics, 232, 229–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.01.029

Chase, J. B. (1972). Retrolental fibroplasia and autistic symptomatology. American Foundation for the Blind.

Cummings, L. (2013). Clinical pragmatics and theory of mind. In A. Capone, F. Lo Piparo, & M. Carapezza (Eds.), Perspectives on linguistic pragmatics (Vol. 2, pp. 23–56). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01014-4_2

De Villiers, J. G., & De Villiers, P. A. (2014). The role of language in theory of mind development. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 313–328. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000037

Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 568–570. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076664

Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Fenici, M. (2017). What is the role of experience in children’s success in the false belief test: Maturation, facilitation, attunement or induction? Mind & Language, 32(3), 308–337. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12145

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.

Frith, C. D. (2012). The role of metacognition in human social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2213–2223. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0123

Gagne, D. L., & Coppola, M. (2017). Visible social interactions do not support the development of false belief understanding in the absence of linguistic input: Evidence from deaf adult homesigners. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 837. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00837

Gagne, D., Goico, S., Pyers, J., & Coppola, M. (2019). False blief understanding requires language experience, but its precur- sor abilities do not. Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development., 256–269. https://www.lingref.com/bucld/43/BUCLD43-20.pdf

Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language, 16(5), 494–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00180

Gentner, D., Özyürek, A., Gürcanli, Ö., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input. Cognition, 127(3), 318–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.003

Glickman, N. S., & Hall, W. C. (2018). Introduction: Culture and disability. In N. S. Glickman & W. C. Hall (Eds.), Language deprivation and deaf mental health (1st ed., pp. 1–23). Routledge.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). Constructing communication by hand. Cognitive Development, 17 (3-4), 1385–1405. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00122-3

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). The resilience of language (1st ed.). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203943267

Goldin-Meadow, S., & Brentari, D. (2017). Gesture, sign, and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15001247

Goldin-Meadow, S., Mylander, C., De Villiers, J., Bates, E., & Volterra, V. (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(3/4), 1. https://doi.org/10.2307/1165838

Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind & Language, 7(1-2), 145–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00202.x

Grice, H. P. (1991). Logic and conversation. In H. P. Grice (Ed.), Studies in the way of words. (pp. 22–40). Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1967)

Gulati, S. (2018). Language deprivation syndrome. In N. S. Glickman & W. C. Hall (Eds.), Language deprivation and deaf mental health (1st ed., pp. 24–53). Routledge.

Hall, W. C. (2017). What you don’t know can hurt you: The risk of language deprivation by impairing sign language devel- opment in deaf children. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(5), 961–965. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-017-2287-y

Hall, W. C., Levin, L. L., & Anderson, M. L. (2017). Language deprivation syndrome: A possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52(6), 761–776. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1351-7

Harris, P. L., De Rosnay, M., & Pons, F. (2005). Language and children’s understanding of mental states. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 69–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00337.x

Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(1), 101–114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98000703

Hunsicker, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). How handshape type can distinguish between nouns and verbs in homesign. Gesture, 13(3), 354–376. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.3.05hun

Keeler, W. R. (1958). Autistic patterns and defective communication in blind children with retrolental fibroplasia. In P. H. H. Hoch & J. Zubin (Eds.), Psychopathology of communication (1st ed., pp. 64–83). Grune & Stratton.

Kegl, J., Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (1999). Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In M. DeGraff (Ed.), Language creation and language change: Creolization, diachrony, and development (1. paperback ed, pp. 179–237). MIT Press.

Lederberg, A. R., Schick, B., & Spencer, P. E. (2013). Language and literacy development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children: Successes and challenges. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 15–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029558

Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in “theory of mind.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 528–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001

Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00597

Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two‐systems account. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12183

Meier, R. P. (2016). Sign language acquisition. In O. H. E. Board (Ed.), Oxford handbook topics in linguistics (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935345.013.19

Meristo, M., Falkman, K. W., Hjelmquist, E., Tedoldi, M., Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2007). Language access and theory of mind reasoning: Evidence from deaf children in bilingual and oralist environments. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1156–1169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1156

Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta‐analysis of the relation between language ability and false‐belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x

Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the united states. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138–163. https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2004.0005

Moeller, M. P., & Schick, B. (2006). Relations between maternal input and theory of mind understanding in deaf children. Child Development, 77(3), 751–766. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00901.x

Morgan, G., & Kegl, J. (2006). Nicaraguan sign language and theory of mind: The issue of critical periods and abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(8), 811–819. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01621.x

Morgan, G., Meristo, M., Mann, W., Hjelmquist, E., Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2014). Mental state language and quality of conversational experience in deaf and hearing children. Cognitive Development, 29, 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.10.002

Napier, J., Leigh, G., & Nann, S. (2007). Teaching sign language to hearing parents of deaf children: An action research process. Deafness & Education International, 9(2), 83–100. https://doi.org/10.1179/146431507790560020

Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621

Perkins, M. (2007). Pragmatic impairment. Cambridge University Press.

Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(3), 459–474. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01303.x

Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126–129. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00119

Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind & Language, 15(1), 123–145. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00126

Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x

Phillips, J., & Norby, A. (2021). Factive theory of mind. Mind & Language, 36(1), 3–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12267

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515– 526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Pyers, J. E., & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20(7), 805–812. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02377.x

Pyers, J. E., Shusterman, A., Senghas, A., Spelke, E. S., & Emmorey, K. (2010). Evidence from an emerging sign language reveals that language supports spatial cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(27), 12116– 12120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914044107

Rakoczy, H. (2017). In defense of a developmental dogma: Children acquire propositional attitude folk psychology around age 4. Synthese, 194(3), 689–707. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0860-8

Rakoczy, H. (2022). Foundations of theory of mind and its development in early childhood. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(4), 223–235. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00037-z

Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2004). Participants don’t need theories: Knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 647–665. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354304046177

Rubio-Fernandez, P. (2019). Theory of mind. In C. Cummins & N. Katsos (Eds.), The oxford handbook of experimental semantics and pragmatics (pp. 524–536). Oxford University Press.

Rubio-Fernandez, P. (2021). Pragmatic markers: The missing link between language and theory of mind. Synthese, 199(1-2), 1125–1158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02768-z

Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science, 24(1), 27–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612447819

Russell, P. A., Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Scott, C., Hunter, N., Banks, J. S., & Macaulay, M. C. (1998). The development of theory of mind in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(6), 903–910. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00390

Santos, S., & Cordes, S. (2022). Math abilities in deaf and hard of hearing children: The role of language in developing number concepts. Psychological Review, 129(1), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000303

Schick, B., De Villiers, P., De Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x

Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in ’theory of mind’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(6), 463–471. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn844

Spelke, E. S. (2022). What babies know: Core knowledgenand composition (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190618247.001.0001

Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind‐reading. Mind & Language, 17(1-2), 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00186

Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Studies in Linguistics: Occasional Papers, 8. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15585746/

Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18(7), 580–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1997). Perspectives on language and communication in autism. In J. L. Matson & P. Sturmey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. (2nd ed., pp. 894–900). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–8498. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304

Westra, E. (2017). Pragmatic development and the false belief task. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 8(2), 235–257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-016-0320-5

Wilson, D. (2005). New directions for research on pragmatics and modularity. Lingua, 115(8), 1129–1146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2004.02.005

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73(3), 768–778. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00437

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Copyright (c) 2023 Endre Begby